Skip to main content

Gender differences in perceived environmental correlates of physical activity

Abstract

Background

Limited research has been conducted on gender differences in perceived environmental correlates of physical activity (PA). The purpose of this study was to explore the potential role of gender in the link between perceived environment and PA.

Methods

Using a telephone-administered survey, data was collected on leisure time physical activity (LTPA), perceptions of the neighbourhood environment, and self-efficacy in a representative sample of 1209 adults from the province of Alberta, Canada. LTPA was regressed on ten measures of perceived neighbourhood environment and self-efficacy in a series of logistic regressions.

Results

Women were more likely than men to perceive their neighbourhood as unsafe to go for walks at night (χ2 = 67.46, p < 0.001) and to report seeing people being active in their neighbourhood (χ2 = 6.73, p < 0.01). Conversely, women were less likely to perceive easy access to places for PA (χ2 = 11.50, p < 0.01) and availability of places to buy things within easy walking distance from home (χ2 = 4.30, p < 0.05). Adjusting for age, education, income, and place of residence, access to places for PA (OR = 2.49) and interesting things to look at in the neighbourhood (OR = 1.94), were associated with higher levels of LTPA in men. Access to places for PA (OR = 2.63) and reporting seeing people being active (OR = 1.50) were associated with increased LTPA among women. After controlling for sociodemographic variables and self-efficacy, the presence of shops and places to buy things within easy walking distance from home (OR = 1.73), interesting things to look at in the neighbourhood (OR = 1.65), and access to places for PA (OR = 1.82) were associated with higher levels of LTPA in men. Among women, no significant relationships were observed between perceived environment and LTPA after adjusting for self-efficacy.

Conclusion

The results provide additional support for the use of models in which gender is treated as a potential moderator of the link between the perceived environment and PA. Further, the results suggest the possibility of differential interventions to increase PA based on factors associated with gender.

Background

A growing body of scientific evidence has brought to public attention the negative effects of physical inactivity on health and the benefits of a physically active lifestyle [1, 2]. Despite the well-documented physical, psychological, and social benefits of regular physical activity (PA) [3, 4], physical inactivity remains pervasive. It is estimated that upwards of two-thirds of the industrialized world does not achieve minimum PA guidelines [5, 2]. Physical inactivity thus constitutes a major public health concern [2] with related social and economic costs [6, 7].

In an effort to solve the PA participation problem, research in the past two decades has employed theoretical perspectives to better understand the factors that enhance and detract from PA participation. In particular, social cognitive models that emphasize the interaction of intrapersonal factors, micro-environmental influences and PA behavior, have gained empirical support. This has allowed researchers to identify cognitive (e.g., efficacy beliefs), affective, and social influences on the individual's choice to be active and to aid in the development of interventions to increase PA levels [8]. Despite being identified as contributing toward behavior change, such individually-focused factors have generally been found to account for a modest proportion of the variance in PA behavior.

In recent years, it has been suggested that wider use of ecological models, which emphasize PA as being the result of multiple influences (i.e., intrapersonal, interpersonal, environmental, organizational and policy levels), hold great promise for addressing the PA participation problem [8–10]. The foregoing is underscored by significant changes observed over the past 50 years in the North American environment which promote decreased PA at the level of the individual. For example, in the United States, while daily vehicle miles traveled per person increased by 224% between 1950 and 2000, the proportion of trips to work by walking decreased 71% between 1960 and 2000 [11]. Transport systems and urban design (e.g., number and quality of pathways and cycleways, availability of buses and bus stops) play an important role in supporting or discouraging PA [12]. Urban sprawl increases distances between differential land use so that roads are more available than cycling paths or sidewalks [13–15].

Consistent with an ecological perspective [9, 16], researchers have attempted to document and understand how different aspects of the physical environment may influence the extent to which individuals are more or less physically active [17–19]. Distribution and quality of local sport and recreational facilities, community clubs and churches, as well as features of the physical environment have all been shown to be associated with PA participation [20, 21].

Researchers have begun to use models that allow selected demographic and personal characteristics such as socioeconomic status [22, 23], gender [24–26] and weight status [27] to act as potential moderators of the effect of the perceived environment rather than as confounding variables. This is also consistent with an ecological perspective, which posits that there are interactions among levels in the system linking individuals with their environments [9, 16].

Understanding the role of gender in the relationship between the perceived environment and PA participation is of particular importance since women typically exhibit lower levels of PA than their male counterparts [24, 28]. Research that has attempted to elucidate gender differences in PA determinants has also revealed that women may face different barriers (e.g., lack of time due to multiple roles, perceptions of safety, and environmental access) than men, which can limit their PA participation [29, 30]. For instance, in a survey of 3,032 U.S. adults [16], psychological factors such as mastery, exercise self-efficacy, and perceived control over health explained educational differences in PA among women.

Despite the emerging interest in the role of the environment and PA participation, few studies have systematically explored differences between women and men in terms of correlates of PA in general, and environmental correlates in particular. Among the exceptions is a recent study [24] that prospectively examined associations of changes in perceptions of local environmental attributes with changes in neighbourhood walking. Results revealed contrasting findings for men and women who reported traffic as less of a problem, with men being 61% less likely to increase walking. Women, on the other hand, were 76% more likely to have done so. Another finding was that men reporting positive changes in aesthetics and convenience were twice as likely to increase their walking. Women who reported positive changes in convenience were more than twice as likely to have increased their walking. In a related study [25], neighborhood walking was associated with high ratings of "aesthetics", "convenience" of facilities and "access" to facilities in men, and with high ratings of "convenience" in women. "Access", on the other hand, was negatively associated with neighborhood walking in women. Finally, in one recent study examining the role of gender on the link between PA and the perceived environment [26], perceptions of safety during day time and convenience (e.g., having shops within walking distance) were positively associated with walking among women. Having a park/open space within walking distance was the only environmental dimension associated with walking in men. Furthermore, there was evidence of substantial differences in perceptions of the environment between genders for both low and high walking groups.

These studies contribute to the literature that documents significant cross-sectional associations of perceived environmental attributes with PA [31]. However, more studies are needed to further elucidate relevant gender differences in PA participation [24]. In particular, little is known yet about how gender may interact with perceptions of the environment in order to influence PA participation. For instance, in relation to PA, it is likely self-efficacy beliefs vary by gender [16] and thus may account for much of any gender differences in perceived environment. Furthermore, no studies have been published on how perceptions of neighbourhood environment relate to PA in Canada. Consequently, the purposes of this study were to determine: a) if gender differences in perceptions of the physical environment exist; b) how these differences may have an impact on leisure time physical activity (LTPA) patterns of individuals in a representative sample of adults from the Province of Alberta; and c) if these gender differences are explained by PA self-efficacy.

Methods

Design and Sample

Participants included a representative sample of 1,209 adults (Males = 600; Females = 609) aged 18 years and over residing in the Province of Alberta, Canada. Data were collected by telephone interview between October 29, 2002, and December 1, 2002. Three separate subsamples represented Edmonton, Calgary, and the rest of the province. A random-digit dialling approach ensured that respondents had an equal chance of being contacted whether or not their household was listed in a telephone directory. Approximately 54% of the total number of valid households responded to the survey. A random sample of 1,209 is considered accurate within +/- 2.8, 19 times out of 20.

Measures

Leisure-Time Physical Activity

The Godin Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire [32] was used to estimate LTPA levels. Self-reported weekly frequencies of strenuous, moderate, and light activities were multiplied by their estimated value in METs (nine, five, and three respectively). Total weekly LTPA was calculated by adding the products of the separate components. Albertans were considered sufficiently physically active if they expended 38 METs a week for men or 35 METs a week for women. These cutoff values were derived based upon the work of several researchers [33–35]. According to Jacobs et al. [34], these values equal 300–400 MET-minutes per day. This number of MET-minutes is equivalent to a weekly energy expenditure of 2,000 kcals [33]. An energy expenditure of 2,000 kcals or more per week is associated with a reduced risk of heart disease [35]. Based on this criterion, we created a dichotomous variable for LTPA: inactive or active.

Perceived environment

Nine questions from the International Physical Activity Prevalence Study Environmental Survey Module [36] were used to assess respondents' perceptions of neighbourhood characteristics that may influence LTPA. The module consists of a series of items that reflect current thinking in assessment of environmental factors for walking and bicycling in one's neighbourhood and for which reliability and validity have been assessed [36]. Since walking (82%) is the primary source of LTPA among Canadians and bicycling is reported by a significant proportion (45%) of the population [37], the environment scale was deemed to be an appropriate measure of neighbourhood influences on LTPA.

Specifically, respondents were asked to indicate how much they agree or disagree with the following items, on a 4-point scale where 1 means strongly disagree and 4 means strongly agree: "Many shops, stores, or other places to buy things I need are within easy walking distance of my home"; "There are sidewalks on most of the streets in my neighbourhood"; "In and around my neighbourhood, there are facilities for bicycling such as special bicycle lanes, bicycle paths, and shared use trails for cyclists and pedestrians"; "My neighbourhood has several free or low cost recreation facilities, such as parks, walking trails, bike paths, playgrounds, and recreation centres"; "The crime rate in my neighbourhood makes it unsafe to go for walks at night"; "There is so much traffic on the streets that it makes it difficult or unpleasant to walk in my neighbourhood"; "I see many people engaging in physical activity in my neighbourhood doing things like walking, jogging, cycling, or playing sports and active games"; "There are many interesting things to look at while walking in my neighbourhood." In addition, respondents were asked to rate on the same 5-point scale their agreement with the following statement: "I have easy access to places where I can get physical activity." For the purposes of this study, the responses to each of the environmental variables were collapsed into categories of disagree (1 or 2) or agree (3 or 4).

Sociodemographic

Sociodemographic variables included age (coded as continuous variable), education (less than secondary, secondary, post-secondary), annual household income (< $30,000;$30,000–60,000; > $60,000), and location (urban, rural).

Self-Efficacy

Based upon previous research [38], a self-efficacy score was derived by averaging the respondents' ratings on three items assessing their confidence in overcoming, respectively, fatigue, bad weather, and time constraints to participating in moderate LTPA. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for these three items was 0.77.

Statistical Analyses

All data were cleaned and edited following standard quality control procedures. The analyses were conducted with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 13.0. Data were weighted to account for the circumstance that the sample sizes of completed interviews for Edmonton, Calgary, and other Alberta, were not proportional to the Alberta population. Specifically, the area samples were multiplied by a weighting factor to account for the slight over representation of residents from Edmonton and Calgary and the slight under representation of residents from other parts of Alberta. However, according to the index of dissimilarity [39], little difference existed between the age groupings of our sample and that of the Alberta population [40]. In addition, the proportion of men and women in our sample was very similar to that of the Canadian and Alberta populations [40]. Thus, the sample adequately reflected the population from which it was drawn.

Descriptive statistics were calculated for sociodemographic variables, self-efficacy, and LTPA. Differences between women and men were tested with chi-square analysis, in the case of categorical variables, and t tests in the case of continuous variables. Gender differences in responses to each environmental item were tested with chi-square analysis. Because PA scores are typically not normally distributed [41], non-parametric procedures were adopted to analyse the LTPA data. Specifically, unconditional logistic regression analyses, with LTPA as the dependent variable, were used to calculate unadjusted and adjusted ORs and the 95% CIs for each environmental variable. To calculate the adjusted ORs, LTPA was regressed on the sociodemographic variables (age, education, income, and location) and, then, self-efficacy was entered on the second step. The decision about which variables to include in the regression analysis was guided by ecological models of PA [9, 16] and other research showing relations between sociodemographic variables and LTPA [42–44]. Conducting separate analyses for males and females allowed us to examine unique patterns of association between the environmental variables and LTPA.

Results

Based on the cut-off values described earlier, 59.9% of men and 54.2% of women were considered physically active. As Table 1 also shows, men displayed significantly higher levels of self-efficacy (t = 6.02, p < 0.001) and had higher annual household income than women (χ2 = 43.46, p < 0.001). Men were also slightly younger than women (t = -2.33, p < 0.05). As seen in Table 2, women were more likely than men to perceive their neighbourhood as unsafe to go for walks at night (χ2 = 67.46, p < 0.001) and to report seeing people being active in their neighbourhood (χ2 = 6.73, p < 0.01). Conversely, women had lower perceptions of easy access to places for PA (χ2 = 11.50, p < 0.01) and of availability of places to buy things within easy walking distance from home (χ2 = 4.30, p < 0.05).

Table 1 Characteristics of Participants
Table 2 Gender Differences in Perceptions of the Physical Environment

The results revealed some interesting differences between men and women in terms of the perceived neighbourhood characteristics that are associated with an increased likelihood of being physically active. As table 3 shows, men who perceived many interesting things to look at while walking in their neighbourhood were 83% more likely to be physically active than those who did not. This association remained statistically significant even after adjusting for sociodemographic variables (OR = 1.94) and self-efficacy (OR = 1.65). When adjusting for sociodemographic variables and self-efficacy, perceptions of presence of shops and places to buy things within easy walking distance from home, were associated with increased LTPA among men as well (OR = 1.73). Among women, perceptions of availability of free or low cost recreational facilities were linked with higher levels of LTPA (OR = 1.53). However, this relationship did not remain statistically significant after adjusting for sociodemographic variables and self-efficacy. Seeing many people engaging in PA in their neighbourhood also increased the likelihood of being physically active in women (OR = 1.50). This association remained statistically significant after adjusting for sociodemographic variables (OR = 1.78) but not after self-efficacy was included in the model.

Table 3 Perceived Environmental Correlates of Being Active Versus Inactive

Perceptions of easy access to places for PA were associated with higher levels of LTPA in both men and women (OR = 2.40 and OR = 2.63, respectively). For men, the relationship was still statistically significant after adjusting for sociodemographic variables (OR = 2.49) and self-efficacy (OR = 1.82). For women, the association was still significant after adjusting for sociodemographic variables (OR = 2.72), but not after self-efficacy was entered in the model.

Discussion

This study explored gender differences in perceptions of the physical environment and in environmental correlates of LTPA in a representative sample of adults from the province of Alberta. Our results are consistent with a growing body of literature that suggests the existence of a link between several dimensions of the perceived environment and self-reported LTPA [20, 31]. Further adding to this literature, our analyses show that other studies may have overlooked an important moderating effect of gender in the relationship between the perceived environment and LTPA.

Unlike previous work concerned with gender differences in the association between PA and environmental variables [26], women and men in our study did not differ, at least to a statistically significant degree, in perceptions of traffic as a potential problem for walking. Consistent with this research [26], women were more concerned than men about the safety of walking at night. Nevertheless, perceptions of low safety were not significantly associated with lower levels of LTPA in women, which is in-line with results from previous studies assessing specific correlates of PA in women [45–47].

Several interesting differences emerged between women and men in terms of the dimensions of the perceived environment that were linked with increased LTPA participation. The perceptions that "there are many interesting things to look at while walking" and that "there are many places to buy things within easy walking distance from home", were significantly associated with higher levels of LTPA in men but not in women. This is somewhat inconsistent with a recent study [24] that found both men and women reporting positive changes over time in aesthetics and convenience in their neighbourhood were at least twice as likely to increase their walking. Likewise, another recent study [26] found that convenience (i.e., availability of shops within walking distance) was positively associated with high levels of walking (>150 mins/week) among women. One possible explanation for our findings is that perceived environments indeed have a differential impact on LTPA according to gender. Another possible explanation is that the role of perceived environment in relation to behaviour may be moderated by cultural and national factors that vary by country and region.

Conversely, since men reported higher levels of LTPA than women, interpretations that PA can affect perceptions of the environment are plausible as well [45]. Offering some support for this hypothesis, we found that men were higher than women in perceptions of easy access to places for PA and of availability of places to buy things within easy walking distance from home. Thus, it could be suggested that individuals who are physically active may be more likely to perceive that they have access to places where they can get PA, or to places where they can buy necessary things, and that they can reach by physically active means (e.g., walking) [48]. Alternatively, these findings may have been due to chance, thus replication of them is necessary. In any case, the unexpected results suggest that the interaction between environmental influences and individual difference characteristics may be more complex than previously thought.

Perceiving that one's neighbourhood has several free or low cost recreation facilities was associated with higher levels of LTPA in women but not in men. This finding makes some sense when taking into account that the average annual household income of the women in our sample was lower than that of men. Studies have found that low amounts of leisure-time PA are strongly associated with low income [49, 50], low education [51], and low socio-economic status [52, 53]. Moreover, the lowest participation rates have been found amongst the poor and women of child-rearing age, many of whom are the same people [54]. In fact, low-income women identify a lack of access to PA in their community as a major factor inhibiting the development of healthy lifestyles for themselves and their families [54]. Thus in the case of our results, having access to places where one can participate in PA for free or at a low cost may have been an incentive (i.e., facilitator) for women to get involved in PA.

Likewise, seeing many people being active in one's neighbourhood was associated with increased levels of LTPA only in women. The positive impact of physically active role models on women's participation patterns has been documented in other studies investigating the influence of characteristics of the neighbourhood environment on PA [45, 46]. Bandura's [55] social cognitive theory explains how role modelling can influence behaviour by enhancing an individual's sense of self-efficacy. Women in our sample had lower self-efficacy to overcome barriers to PA than men. Consistent with previous research showing the importance of self-efficacy on women's LTPA [16, 47], this may help explain why physically active role models seem to have been more influential for women in our study. However, the circumstance that neither having access to places where one can participate in PA for free or at neither a low cost, nor seeing many people being active in one's neighbourhood reached statistical significance when adjusted by self-efficacy points to an interesting phenomenon. It seems indeed as if self-efficacy to overcome barriers to PA participation "in spite of" an environment that may facilitate or hinder participation was more influential among women in our sample than self-efficacy "because of" a supportive environment (i.e., one in which role models and low cost facilities are available).

Having easy access to places for PA was the environmental dimension most strongly associated with LTPA in both men (even after adjusting for sociodemographic factors and self-efficacy) and women (even after adjusting for sociodemographics). Once again, the influential role of self-efficacy on women's LTPA in our sample was evidenced when adjusting for this variable had the effect of eliminating the statistical significance of the association. Access to places for PA has previously been linked with increased levels of PA in some studies [48, 56] but not in others [46, 47]. As Huston et al. [48] have discussed, this measure may appear more strongly related with PA simply because it provides a more global indicator of whether or not an individual has access to suitable places for PA, whether in their neighbourhood or not. Alternatively, it could be that individuals who are physically active may be more likely to perceive that they have access to places or facilities where they can get PA [48]. As our study shows, accounting for gender differences (e.g., in self-efficacy levels) may help to partially explain the discrepancy in results across studies assessing the impact of perceptions of access to places for PA. Thus, interventions should be designed to increase women's self-efficacy to participate in PA when environmental supports may not be readily available. Our results also suggest that in order to encourage women to become more physically active, interventions could be designed to increase awareness and use of environmental supports [57] such as places were people can engage at low or no cost in PA while seeing socially and economically similar others doing the same.

This study has several limitations. First, our data is cross-sectional and therefore we cannot make causal inferences. We also relied on a telephone survey format, which may have resulted in some segments of the population (e.g., low income individuals lacking access to telephones) being underrepresented. Additionally, we used self-report measures of both LTPA and perceptions of the physical environment. It is thus possible that the data may have been subject to biases. Since it is not clear yet whether the actual or perceived environment is more influential [9, 45, 58], it is important that future studies include assessments of both dimensions in their designs.

Conclusion

To date, few studies have explored gender differences in perceptions of the environment and how these differences may have an impact on PA patterns of individuals. We found several gender relevant differences in the ways individuals perceive their physical environments and on the dimensions of these environments that are associated with LTPA. Thus, the results from this study add to the knowledge base of gender differences in environmental correlates of PA. Our findings provide further support for the need of using models in which gender is treated as a potential moderator of the link between the perceived environment and PA. Further, these results suggest the possibility of designing interventions to increase PA that address differentially both structural (e.g., income disparities) and social cognitive (e.g., self-efficacy) factors typically associated with gender.

References

  1. Ruhling RO: Why not exercise?. Quest. 2004, 56: 208-225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. US Department of Health and Human Services: Physical activity and health: A report of the Surgeon General. 1996, Atlanta Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bouchard C, Shephard RJ, Stephens T: Physical activity fitness and health: International proceedings and consensus statement. 1994, Champaign: Human Kinetics

    Google Scholar 

  4. Gauvin L, Spence JC, Anderson S: Exercise and psychological well-being in the adult population: Reality or wishful thinking?. Textbook of medicine exercise nutrition and health. Edited by: Rippe JM. 1999, Malden: Blackwell, 957-966.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Oja P: Descriptive epidemiology of health-related physical activity and fitness. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1995, 214: 303-312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Colditz GA: Economic costs of obesity and inactivity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1999, 663-667. 10.1097/00005768-199911001-00026. Suppl

  7. Katzmarzyk PT, Gledhill N, Shephard RJ: The economic burden of physical inactivity in Canada. CMAJ. 2000, 163: 1435-1440.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. King AC, Stokols D, Talen E, Brassington GS, Killingsworth : Theoretical approaches to the promotion of physical activity: Forging a transdisciplinary paradigm. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 15-25. 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00470-1. Suppl 1

  9. Spence JC, Lee RE: Toward a comprehensive model of physical activity. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2003, 4: 7-24. 10.1016/S1469-0292(02)00014-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Saelens BE, Sallis JF, Frank LD: Environmental correlates of walking and cycling: Findings from the transportation, urban design, and planning literature. Ann Behav Med. 2003, 25: 80-91. 10.1207/S15324796ABM2502_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Brownson RC, Boehmer TK, Luke DA: Declining rates of physical activity in the United States: What are the contributos?. Ann Rev Public Health. 2005, 26: 421-443. 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Frank LD, Engelke PO, Schmid TL: Health and community design: The impact of the built environment on physical activity. 2003, Washington: Island Press

    Google Scholar 

  13. Ewing R, Schmid T, Killingsworth R, Zlot A, Raudenbush S: Relationship between urban sprawl and physical activity, obesity, and morbidity. Am J Health Prom. 2003, 18: 47-57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Frank LD, Andresen MA, Schmid TL: Obesity relationships with community design, physical activity, and time spent in cars. Am J Prev Med. 2004, 27: 87-96. 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.04.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Frank LD, Schmid TL, Sallis JF, Chapman J, Saelens BE: Linking objectively measured physical activity with objectively measured urban form Findings from SMARTRAQ. Am J Prev Med. 2005, 28 (Suppl 2): 117-125. 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Grzywacz JG, Marks NF: Social inequalities and exercise during adulthood: Toward an ecological perspective. J Health Soc Behav. 2001, 42: 202-220.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Bauman A, Smith B, Stoker L, Bellew B, Booth M: Geographical influences upon physical activity participation: Evidence of a 'coastal effect'. Aust New Zeal J Public Health. 1999, 23: 322-324.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Corti B, Donovan RJ, Holman CDJ: Factors influenceing the use of physical activity facilities: Results from qualitative research. Health Prom J Aust. 1997, 7: 16-21.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Rutten A, Abel T, Kannas L, Von Lengerke T, Luschen G, Diaz JA, Vinck J, Van der Zee J: Self-reported physical activity, public health and perceived environment: Results from a comparative European study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001, 55: 139-146. 10.1136/jech.55.2.139.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Humpel N, Owen N, Leslie E: Environmental factors associated with adults' participation in physical activity: A review. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 22: 188-199. 10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00426-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Sallis JF, Kraft K, Linton LS: How the environment shapes physical activity: A transdisciplinary research agenda. Am J Prev Med. 2002, 22: 208-10.1016/S0749-3797(01)00435-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Parks SE, Housemann RA, Brownson RC: Differential correlates of physical activity in urban and rural adults of various socioeconomic backgrounds in the United States. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003, 57: 29-35. 10.1136/jech.57.1.29.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. van Lenthe FJ, Brug J, Mackenback JP: Neighbourhood inequalities in physical activity: The role of neighbourhood attractiveness, proximity to local facilities and safety in the Netherlands. Soc Sci Med. 2005, 60: 763-775. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.013.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Humpel N, Marshall AL, Leslie E, Bauman A, Owen N: Changes in neighborhood walking are related to changes in perceptions of environmental attributes. Ann Behav Med. 2004, 27: 60-67. 10.1207/s15324796abm2701_8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Humpel N, Owen N, Leslie E, Marshall AL, Bauman AE, Sallis JF: Associations of location and perceived environmental attributes with walking in neighborhoods. Am J Health Prom. 2004, 18: 239-242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Foster C, Hillsdon M, Thorogood M: Environmental perceptions and walking in English adults. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004, 58: 924-928. 10.1136/jech.2003.014068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Blanchard CM, McGannon K, Spence JC, Rhodes RE, Nehl E, Baker F, Bostwick J: Social ecological correlates of physical activity in normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals. Int J Obes. 2005, 29: 720-726. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802927.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Pinto BM, Marcus BH, Clark MM: Promoting physical activity in women: The new challenges. Am J Prev Med. 1996, 12: 395-400.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Eyler AA: Personal, social, and environmental correlates of physical activity in rural Midwestern white women. Am J Prev Med. 2003, 86-92. 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00169-7. Suppl 1

  30. Jaffee L, Lutter JM, Rex J, Hawkes C, Buccacio P: Incentives and barriers to PA for working women. Am J Health Prom. 1999, 13: 215-218.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Duncan MJ, Spence JC, Mummery WK: Perceived environment and physical activity: A meta-analysis of selected environmental characteristics. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2005, 2: 11-10.1186/1479-5868-2-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Godin G, Shephard RJ: A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Can J Appl Sport Sci. 1985, 10: 141-146.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Elosúa R, García M, Aguilar A, Molina L, Covas MI, Marrugat J: Validation of the Minnesota Leisure Time Physical Activity Questionnaire In Spanish Women. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2000, 32: 1431-1437. 10.1097/00005768-200008000-00011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Jacobs DR, Ainsworth BE, Hartman TJ, Leon AS: A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical activity questionnaires. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1993, 25: 81-91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Paffenbarger RS, Wing AL, Hyde RT: Physical activity as an index of heart attack risk in college alumni. Am J Epidemiol. 1978, 108: 161-175.

    Google Scholar 

  36. International Physical Activity Prevalence Study Environmental Survey Module.http://www-rohan.sdsu.edu/faculty/sallis/IPAQIPS.pdf

  37. Candian Fitness Lifestyle Research Institute: Physical Activity Monitor. 2000, Ottawa, ON

    Google Scholar 

  38. Rodgers WM, Sullivan MJL: Task, coping, and scheduling self-efficacy in relation to frequency of physical activity. J Appl Soc Psych. 2001, 31: 741-753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Duncan OD, Duncan B: Residential distribution and occupational stratification. Am J Sociol. 1955, 60: 493-10.1086/221609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Statistics Canada: . 2001 Census of Canada. 2002, Ottawa ON,http://www12.statcan.ca/english/census01/home/index.cfm

    Google Scholar 

  41. Thomas JR, Thomas K: Physical activity data: Odd distributions yield strange answers. Physical activity assessments for health-related research. Edited by: Welk GJ. 2002, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 73-80.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Clark DO: Racial and educational differences in physical activity among older adults. Gerontologist. 1995, 35: 472-480.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Potvin L, Gauvin L, Nguyen NM: Prevalence of stages of change for physical activity in rural, suburban and inner-city communities. JComm Health. 1997, 22: 1-13. 10.1023/A:1025161522683.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Powell LM, Slater S, Chaloupka FJ: The relationship between community physical activity settings and race, ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Evidence-Based Prev Med. 2004, 1: 135-144.

    Google Scholar 

  45. King AC, Castro C, Wilcox S, Eyler AA, Sallis JF, Brownson RC: Personal and environmental factors associated with physical inactivity among different racial-ethnic groups of U.S. middle-aged and older-aged women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19: 354-364. 10.1037//0278-6133.19.4.354.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Wilcox S, Castro C, King AC, Housemann R, Brownson RC: Determinants of leisure time physical activity in rural activity compared with urban older and ethnically diverse women in the United States. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2000, 54: 667-672. 10.1136/jech.54.9.667.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Eyler AA, Matson-Koffman D, Young DR, Wilcox S, Wilbur J, Thompson JL, Sanderson B, Evenson KR: Quantitative study of correlates of physical activity in women from diverse racial/ethnic groups: The Women's Cardiovascular Health Network Project – summary and conclusions. Am J Prev Med. 2003, 93-103. 10.1016/S0749-3797(03)00170-3. Suppl 1

  48. Huston SL, Evenson KR, Bors P, Gizlice Z: Neighborhood environment, access to places for activity, and leisure-time physical activity in a diverse North Carolina population. Am J Health Prom. 2003, 18: 58-69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Stachenko SJ, Reeder BA, Lindsay E, Donovan C, Lessard R: Smoking prevalence and associated risk factors in Canadian adults. Canadian Medical Association Journal. 1992, Canadian Heart Health Surveys Research, 146: 1989-1996.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Steenland K: Passive smoking and the risk of heart disease. JAMA. 1992, 267: 94-99. 10.1001/jama.267.1.94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Sternfeld B, Ainsworth BE, Quesenberry JCP: Physical activity patterns in a diverse population of women. Prev Med. 1999, 28: 313-323. 10.1006/pmed.1998.0470.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Blanksby B, Anderson M, Douglas GA: Recreational patterns, body composition and socioeconomic status of Western secondary school students. Ann Human Biol. 1996, 23: 101-112.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Mensink GBM, Losse N, Oomen CM: Physical activity and its association with other lifestyle factors. Eur J Epidemiol. 1997, 13: 771-778. 10.1023/A:1007474220830.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Frisby W, Crawford S, Dorer T: Reflections on participatory action research: The case of low-income women accessing local physical activity services. J Sport Management. 1997, 11: 8-28.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Bandura A: The explanatory and predictive scope of self-efficacy theory. J Soc Clin Psychol. 1986, 4: 359-373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Brownson RC, Baker EA, Housemann RA, Brennan LK, Bacak SJ: Environmental and policy determinants of physical activity in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2001, 91: 1995-2003.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Addy CL, Wilson DK, Kirtland KA, Ainsworth BE, Sharpe P, Kimsey D: Associations of perceived social and physical environmental supports with physical activity and walking behavior. Am J Public Health. 2004, 94: 440-443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Sallis JF, Johnson MF, Calfas KJ, Caparosa S, Nichols JF: Assessing perceived physical environmental variables that may influence physical activity. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1997, 68: 345-351.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John C Spence.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

EGB participated in the conception and design of the study, performed the statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. JCS participated in the conception and design of the study, took a coordination role, and helped to draft the manuscript. KRM coordinated the design of the survey and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bengoechea, E.G., Spence, J.C. & McGannon, K.R. Gender differences in perceived environmental correlates of physical activity. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2, 12 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-2-12

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-2-12

Keywords