Skip to main content

Portion size: review and framework for interventions

Abstract

The prevalence of overweight and obesity has increased. A strong environmental factor contributing to the obesity epidemic is food portion size. This review of studies into the effects of portion size on energy intake shows that increased food portion sizes lead to increased energy intake levels. Important mechanisms explaining why larger portions are attractive and lead to higher intake levels are value for money and portion distortion. This review also shows that few intervention studies aiming to reverse the negative influence of portion size have been conducted thus far, and the ones that have been conducted show mixed effects. More intervention studies targeted at portion size are urgently needed. Opportunities for further interventions are identified and a framework for portion size interventions is proposed. Opportunities for intervention include those targeted at the individual as well as those targeted at the physical, economic, political and socio-cultural environment.

Introduction

Overweight and obesity are increasing problems in western societies. Environmental factors contribute to the obesity epidemic [1] by promoting energy intake and limiting opportunities for energy expenditure [2]. A strong environmental factor influencing energy intake is food portion size [3–6]. Although research on the actual development of portion sizes is limited, it is clear that portion sizes have increased over the past decades [7–11]. Studies have been conducted in the United States [7–10] and in Denmark [11]. These studies show that, since the 1970s, portion sizes of especially high energy-dense foods, eaten inside as well as outside the home, have increased. This accounts for both amorphous foods and foods served in units [9]. Fast-food restaurants, for example, have shown a trend over the last decades to supersize their portions, and have introduced large and mega meals [9–11]. Another example is the increased package sizes of products sold in supermarkets, such as sugar-sweetened beverages [11].

Food portions in the United States tend to be larger than in Europe. However, in Europe, portion sizes have also increased [5, 10, 11]. Increased portion sizes may lead to increased energy intake levels. Studies on interventions that aim to reverse this trend are scarce, and urgently needed. In this article, firstly we review the effects of portion size on energy intake, followed by possible explanations for this relationship. Next, we assess the currently available interventions and their effectiveness. To conclude, we identify further opportunities for interventions aimed at portion size and propose a framework for portion size interventions.

Methods

Search strategy

For this review, firstly we asked: What is the effect of portion size on energy intake? Secondly, we assessed the effects of currently available portion size interventions on food intake. Studies were identified using the PUBMED database, the Cochrane Library and the Web of Science (ISI). The following keywords were used for the first question: 'portion size'; 'energy intake'; 'food intake'; and 'food consumption'. For the second part, the keywords 'portion size'; 'intervention'; or 'programme' were used. Furthermore, studies were also identified based on references of the found articles.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Only studies with adults as research population were included in this review. However, intervention studies with a mixed study sample, but consisting mainly of adults, were also included. Studies with less than 20 subjects were excluded in all cases. Food intake had to be an outcome measure of the study to be included in the review. More specifically, food intake of the product whose portion size was manipulated had to be an outcome measure (for example, instead of only the food intake of a non-manipulated subsequent meal). In addition, for the second part, intervention studies that used food selection as an outcome measure were also included.

For the first question, studies varying only the package format and not the actual portion size were excluded (for example 30 grams in a small package versus 30 grams in a large package). Regarding study design, between subjects designs were included as well as within subjects cross-over designs for the first research question. Since studies into the effectiveness of interventions aimed at portion size are very scarce, no further requirements were defined regarding research design for the second question.

Portion size and energy intake

Do increased portion sizes lead to increased energy intake levels? Thirteen studies met the inclusion criteria and investigated this relationship, mostly using a within subject's cross-over design (see Table 1) [12–24]. The larger portion sizes used in the studies varied from 125% of the control portion to up to 500% the control portion, but most studies investigated portion sizes between the control size and twice the control size. All studies showed that people's energy intake increases when offered a larger portion. This also accounts for food with an unfavourable perceived taste, i.e. stale popcorn [23].

Table 1 Studies into the effects of portion size on food intake

Effects of at least 30% higher consumption levels due to portion size are reported frequently [12, 16, 17, 19, 22–24], with larger effects for larger portion sizes (see Table 1). The effects have been shown for a variety of foods, such as macaroni [17], a pre-packaged snack [18], beverages [14] or popcorn offered in a cinema setting [22]. Larger effects are found for men [18–20], compared to women. If the increase in portion size was combined with a higher energy density, even larger effects on energy intake were observed [15]. Furthermore, research showed that the effects of portion size can persist over several days, with no indication of meal-to-meal compensation [20, 21]. Rolls et al [21] showed that the average increase in caloric intake owing to 50% larger portions did not decline over a period of eleven days, and resulted in a cumulative increase of, on average, more than 4600 kcal during the eleven-day research period.

Explanations: Portion distortion and value for money

Important factors found in the literature, explaining why people buy and eat larger portion sizes than they actually need, are the notions of 'value for money' and 'portion distortion'. Larger portions are made attractive by offering more value for money, i.e. having a lower price per unit. Lower unit costs also explain why larger package or portion sizes lead to a higher user volume [6, 25, 26]. Based on qualitative focus group interviews, it seems that consumers experience the lower price per unit in case of larger portions as a natural pattern and are used to it (March 2007; unpublished data).

Next, continuous exposure to larger food portion sizes contributes to 'portion distortion' among consumers [11, 27]. People experiencing portion distortion perceive larger portion sizes as an appropriate amount to consume at a single occasion [28]. It also refers to the fact that individuals do not realize that their portion size commonly exceeds the serving size [29]. With respect to portion distortion, a number of aspects are relevant. First, larger portions have become standard and, as a consequence, consumers have difficulty selecting amounts of food that are appropriate for their weight and activity levels [30]. Second, market place portions differ increasingly from recommended standard portion sizes defined by federal agencies [30]. In fact, market place portions are often three to four times larger than the recommended portion size, while consumers perceive market place portions as standard portions [31].

Several studies have shown that people tend to select substantially larger portions than the recommended portion sizes [28, 29, 32, 33]. Third, labels on food packaging are not always clear with respect to the serving size. Sometimes, unrealistic small serving sizes are used on food packages in order to give consumers a positive impression about the number of servings in one package and the caloric content [29]. Similarly, using the terms 'small', 'medium' and 'large' also creates confusion, as people's interpretation of these terms differs [34]. A fourth factor relevant in portion distortion is the 'unit bias' people might experience. Geier et al [35] define unit bias as 'a sense that a single entity is the appropriate amount to engage, consume or consider'. The size of the unit or package sets a consumption norm for consumers, which might not be an appropriate norm in accordance with food recommendations [35, 36]. Many people interpret package size as a single serving size and are unaware of the fact that a package contains multiple servings [37]. Fifth, and finally, tableware might also contribute to portion distortion, although study results are inconclusive as yet. The vertical-horizontal illusion is well known: people only use the vertical dimension to estimate portion size [38]. Also, it seems that people serve themselves more food if using a larger bowl [39, 40]. However, for plate sizes, this finding could not be replicated [41].

Once larger portions have been selected because of the value for money and portion distortion principle, passive over-consumption is likely to occur. In particular, people tend to overeat palatable, high energy-dense (e.g. high in fat) foods, without deliberate intention [42].

Interventions

Intervention studies

Is it possible to reverse the trend towards larger portions and the consequently higher energy intake levels? Despite the fact that a broad range of interventions aimed at portion size have been suggested in the literature, very few intervention studies aimed at portion size have been conducted thus far. Only five studies were found that met the inclusion criteria for the intervention studies (see Table 2) [43–47]. It must be noted that two of these intervention studies were conducted among relatively small samples [46, 47], with 24 and 33 respondents respectively. Also, three of these five studies were conducted among a relatively young and healthy population; i.e. college students [43, 45, 46]. Few other studies describing an intervention aimed at portion size and targeted at adults were found. However, these studies did not have food intake or selection as their outcome measurement, or were not evaluated at all [27, 48–52]. These interventions were developed to target the improvement of consumers' portion size estimation skills, and to educate people about appropriate portion sizes [27, 48–52]. Of the five included studies, two were targeted at reducing portion sizes [45, 46].

Table 2 Studies into the effectiveness of interventions aimed at portion size

A portion size reduction of 25%, studied in a laboratory setting, was effective in decreasing energy intake. Moreover, reducing the energy density, while keeping the same portion size, led to a larger decrease in energy intake [46]. However, a study into reducing portion sizes in a college setting showed no effects on total energy intake [45]. Lieux and Manning [45] studied an intervention based on limiting portion sizes of hot entrées in a dining facility at an American university. Portion size labelling or portion size information seems to be ineffective in decreasing energy intake [44, 47]. In Harnack's study [44], portion size labelling was combined with another pricing structure (i.e. standardized pricing: the same price per gram for small and large portions; instead of value size pricing: the per unit cost decreases as portion size increases), which was also ineffective in changing the caloric intake. As customers are not used to standardized pricing, it might be that repeated exposure is necessary in order to achieve an effect [44].

Suggestions for individual and environmental interventions aimed at portion size

Several suggestions for interventions aimed at portion size have been given in the literature (see Table 3). Interventions can be targeted at the individual, comprising the education of consumers. Educational interventions should address awareness and teach behavioural strategies for portion control at home as well as, for example, in restaurants [4, 7, 10, 11, 28, 29, 31, 53, 54] (see Table 1 for more details regarding the suggested interventions).

Table 3 Opportunities for interventions aimed at portion size

Further, environmental interventions are important as well, since increased portion sizes are part of a changed food environment. Physical, economic, political and socio-cultural aspects of the environment can be distinguished according to the ANGELO-grid (ANalysis Grid for Environments Linked to Obesity) [1]. The physical environment refers to available options to make a healthy choice; the economic environment refers to the cost of healthy choices; the political environment refers to rules and regulations that may influence healthy choices; and the socio-cultural environment refers to social and cultural norms influencing healthy choices [1, 55]. Interventions aimed at portion size can be put in place in all four types of environment (see Table 1). Decreasing portion sizes, or a wider range of available portion sizes, are examples of interventions aimed at the physical environment [10, 23, 28, 29, 33, 53, 54, 56, 57]. Pricing strategies to make smaller portions more attractive aim to alter the economic environment [4, 26, 53, 54]. Possible interventions in the political environment are portion size requirements in certain settings, such as schools, or formulating realistic serving size standards [33, 48, 53, 58]. Finally, interventions in the socio-cultural environment could be directed at chefs in restaurants, aimed at changing their knowledge, attitudes and skills regarding portion size and thereby influencing their customers' food consumption (see Table 3 for more details regarding the suggested interventions).

Portion size intervention framework

The suggested interventions target different aspects of portion size, which contribute to a higher energy intake mentioned before (see 'Portion distortion and value for money'). Figure 1 shows a framework for portion size interventions. The underlying factors causing portion distortion can be diminished by means of environmental interventions, mainly in the physical and political environment. Alongside this, education of consumers may help them to cope with an environment filled with portion distortive factors. Furthermore, proposed pricing strategies direct the value for money principle. Most of the suggested interventions are targeted at the selection of food, which is of great value because once a larger portion is selected, over-consumption is very likely to occur. Yet, by decreasing the energy density of food products and meals, one is still able to select a larger volume, while having fewer consequences for energy intake. Since value for money, as well as portion distortion phenomena, are less important using this strategy, this might be a promising alternative and attractive to both consumers and retailers.

Figure 1
figure 1

Framework for portion size interventions

Feasibility

The feasibility of interventions targeted at portion size depends on the willingness of both consumers and point-of-purchase settings to accept these interventions. A qualitative study into consumer attitudes about portion size interventions indicated that consumers had particularly favourable attitudes towards a larger variety of portion sizes and pricing strategies, followed by labelling interventions (March 2007; unpublished data). Another qualitative study using semi-structured individual interviews with representatives of point-of-purchase settings, showed that most interventions aimed at portion size can be considered as innovative. Nonetheless, offering a larger variety of portion sizes and portion-size labelling were perceived as especially feasible interventions [59]. Also, O'Dougherty et al [60] showed that a third of fast-food restaurant patrons favoured a law requiring restaurants to change their pricing strategies and offer lower prices for smaller portions, instead of more value for money for larger portions.

Conclusion

Portion sizes seem to have increased considerably over the last few decades. It is important to continue studying trends in actual portion size development, since not many studies are currently available. The same applies to studies into the long-term effects of increased portion sizes. This review summarizes the available evidence, demonstrating that increased portion sizes lead to increased energy intake levels. Important factors explaining why larger portions are attractive, and why they lead to higher intake levels, are related to value for money and portion distortion. Only few intervention studies have been conducted to target portion size. Interventions that have been tested were directed mainly towards the physical environment, namely portion size reduction and portion size labelling or information. So far, interventions have shown mixed effects. Intervention studies are urgently needed, to find out what type of interventions, targeted at portion size, are effective, in what setting, and among which target groups. These studies should focus on educational programmes, but also on interventions in the physical, economic, politic and socio-cultural environments.

References

  1. Swinburn B, Egger G, Raza F: Dissecting obesogenic environments: The development and application of a framework for identifying and prioritizingenvironmental iinterventions for obesity. Prev Med. 1999, 29: 563-570. 10.1006/pmed.1999.0585.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Drewnowski A, Rolls BJ: How to modify the food environment. J Nutr. 2005, 135: 898-899.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Giskes K, van Lenthe F, Brug J, Mackenbach JP, Turrell G: Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: The contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med. 2007, 45 (1): 41-8. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.04.007.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Ledikwe JH, Ello-Martin JA, Rolls BJ: Portion sizes and the obesity epidemic. J Nutr. 2005, 135: 905-909.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Rozin P, Kabnick K, Pete E, Fischler C, Shields C: The ecology of eating: Smaller portion sizes in France than in the United States help explain the French paradox. Psychol Sc. 2003, 14 (5): 450-454. 10.1111/1467-9280.02452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Swinburn BA, Caterson I, Seidell JC, James WPT: Diet, nutrition and the prevention of excess weight gain and obesity. Public Health Nutr. 2004, 7 (1A): 123-146.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Nielsen SJ, Popkin BM: Patterns and trends in food portion sizes, 1977–1998. JAMA. 2003, 289 (4): 450-453. 10.1001/jama.289.4.450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Smiciklas-Wright H, Mitchell DC, Mickle SJ, Goldman JD, Cook A: Foods commonly eaten in the United States, 1989–1991 and 1994–1996: Are portion sizes changing?. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003, 103 (1): 41-47. 10.1053/jada.2003.50000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Young LR, Nestle M: The contribution of expanding portion sizes to the US obesity epidemic. Am J Public Health. 2002, 92 (2): 246-249. 10.2105/AJPH.92.2.246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Young LR, Nestle M: Portion sizes and obesity: Responses of fast-food companies. J Public Health Pol. 2007, 28: 238-248. 10.1057/palgrave.jphp.3200127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Matthiessen J, Fagt S, Biltoft-Jensen A, Beck AM, Ovesen L: Size makes a difference. Public Health Nutr. 2003, 6 (1): 65-72. 10.1079/PHN2002361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dilliberti N, Bordi PL, Conklin MT, Roe LS, Rolls BJ: Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal. Obes Res. 2004, 12 (3): 562-568. 10.1038/oby.2004.64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Fisher JO, Arreola A, Birch LL, Rolls BJ: Portion size effects on daily energy intake in low-income Hispanic and African American children and their mothers. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007, 86: 1709-1716.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Flood JE, Roe LS, Rolls BJ: The effects of increased beverage portion size on energy intake at a meal. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006, 106: 1984-1990. 10.1016/j.jada.2006.09.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kral TVE, Roe LS, Rolls BJ: Combined effects of energy density ad portion size on energy intake in women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004, 79 (6): 962-968.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Raynor HA, Wing RR: Package unit and amount of food: Do both influence intake?. Obesity. 2007, 15 (9): 2311-2319. 10.1038/oby.2007.274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Rolls BJ, Morris EL, Roe LS: Portion size of food affects energy intake in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002, 76: 1207-1213.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Kral TVE, Meengs JS, Wall DE: Increasing the portion size of a packaged snack increases energy intake in men and women. Appetite. 2004, 42: 63-69. 10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00117-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS, Wall DE: Increasing the portion size of a sandwich increases energy intake. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004, 104 (3): 367-372. 10.1016/j.jada.2003.12.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS: Larger portion sizes lead to sustained increase in energy intake over 2 days. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006, 106: 543-549. 10.1016/j.jada.2006.01.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS: The effect of large portion sizes on energy intake is sustained for 11 days. Obesity. 2007, 15 (6): 1535-1543. 10.1038/oby.2007.182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Wansink B, Park SB: At the movies: How external cues and perceived taste impact consumption volume. Food Qual Prefer. 2001, 12: 69-74. 10.1016/S0950-3293(00)00031-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Wansink B, Kim J: Bad popcorn in big buckets: Portion size can influence intake as much as taste. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2005, 37: 242-245. 10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60278-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Wansink B, Painter JE, North J: Bottomless bowls: Why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Res. 2005, 13 (1): 93-100. 10.1038/oby.2005.12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. French SA: Public health strategies for dietary change: Schools and workplaces. J Nutr. 2005, 135 (4): 910-912.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Wansink B: Can package size accelerate usage volume?. J Marketing. 1996, 60: 1-14. 10.2307/1251838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Penisten M, Litchfield R: Nutrition education delivered at the state fair: Are your portions in proportion?. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2004, 36: 275-277. 10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60392-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Schwartz J, Byrd-Bredbenner C: Portion distortion: Typical portion sizes selected by young adults. J Am Diet Assoc. 2006, 106: 1412-1418. 10.1016/j.jada.2006.06.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Bryant R, Dundes L: Portion distortion: A study of college students. J Consum Aff. 2005, 39 (2): 399-408. 10.1111/j.1745-6606.2005.00021.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Young LR, Nestle M: Expanding portion sizes in the US marketplace: Implications for nutrition counselling. J Am Diet Assoc. 2003, 103 (2): 231-234. 10.1053/jada.2003.50027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Hogbin M, Hess M: Public confusion over food portions and servings. J Am Diet Assoc. 1999, 99 (10): 1209-1211. 10.1016/S0002-8223(99)00297-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Burger K, Kern M, Coleman K: Characteristics of self-selected portion size in young adults. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007, 107: 611-618. 10.1016/j.jada.2007.01.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Condrasky M, Ledikwe JH, Flood JE, Rolls BJ: Chefs' Opinions of Restaurant Portion Sizes. Obesity. 2007, 15 (8): 2086-2094. 10.1038/oby.2007.248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Young LR, Nestle M: Variation in perceptions of a "medium" food portion: Implications for dietary guidance. J Am Diet Assoc. 1998, 98 (4): 458-459. 10.1016/S0002-8223(98)00103-5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Geier AB, Rozin P, Doros G: A new heuristic that helps explain the effect of portion size on food Intake. Psychol Sci. 2006, 17 (6): 521-525. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01738.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Wansink B: Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr. 2004, 24: 455-479. 10.1146/annurev.nutr.24.012003.132140.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Pelletier AL, Chang WW, Delzell JJ, McCall JW: Patients' understanding and use of snackfood package nutrition labels. JABFP. 2004, 17 (5): 319-323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. van Ittersum K, Wansink B: Do Children Really Prefer Large Portions? Visual illusions bias their estimates and intake. J Am Diet Assoc. 2007, 107 (7): 1107-1110. 10.1016/j.jada.2007.05.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Wansink B, van Ittersum K, Painter JE: Ice cream illusions. Bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes. Am J Prev Med. 2006, 31 (3): 240-243. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Wansink B, Cheney MM: Super bowls: Serving bowl size and food consumption. JAMA. 2005, 293 (14): 1727-1728. 10.1001/jama.293.14.1727.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Halverson KH, Meengs JS: Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite. 2007, 49: 652-660. 10.1016/j.appet.2007.04.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Blundell JE, Macdiarmid JI: Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: Satiation, satiety, and patterns of eating. J Am Diet Assoc. 1997, 97S (suppl): S63-S69. 10.1016/S0002-8223(97)00733-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Antonuk B, Block LG: The effect of single serving versus entire package nutritional information on consumption norms and actual consumption of a snack food. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2006, 38: 365-370. 10.1016/j.jneb.2006.05.016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Harnack LJ, French SA, Oakes JM, Story MT, Jeffery RW, Rydell SA: Effects of calorie labelling and value size pricing on fast food meal choices: Results from an experimental trial. IJBNPA. 2008, 5: 63.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Lieux EM, Manning CK: Evening meals selected by college students: Impact of the foodservice system. J Am Diet Assoc. 1992, 92 (5): 560.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Rolls BJ, Roe LS, Meengs JS: Reductions in portion size and energy density of foods are additive and lead to sustained decreases in energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006, 83: 11-17.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Ueland O, Cardello AV, Merrill EP, Lesher LL: Effect of portion size information on food intake. J Am Diet Ass. 2009, 109 (1): 124-127. 10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Ayala GX: An experimental evaluation of a group- versus computer-based intervention to improve food portion size estimation skills. Health Educ Res. 2006, 21 (1): 133-145. 10.1093/her/cyh049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Brown RM, Oler CH: A food display assignment and handling food models improves accuracy of college student's estimates of food portions. J Am Diet Assoc. 2000, 100 (9): 1063-1064. 10.1016/S0002-8223(00)00309-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Camelon KM, Hadell K: The Plate Model: A visual method of teaching meal planning. J Am Diet Assoc. 1998, 98 (10): 1155-10.1016/S0002-8223(98)00267-3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Dagget LM, Rigdon KL: A computer-assisted instructional program for teaching portion size versus serving size. J Community Health Nurs. 2006, 23 (1): 29-35. 10.1207/s15327655jchn2301_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Riley WT, Beasley J, Sowell A, Behar A: Effects of a web-based food portion training program on food portion estimation. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2007, 39: 70-76. 10.1016/j.jneb.2006.08.028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Rolls BJ: The supersizing of America. Nutr Today. 2003, 38 (2): 42-53. 10.1097/00017285-200303000-00004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ello-Martin JA, Ledikwe JH, Rolls BJ: The influence of food portion size and energy density on energy intake: implications for weight management. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005, 82 (sSuppl): S236-S241.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Brug J, van Lenthe F: Environmental determinants and interventions for physical activity, nutrition and smoking: A review. 2005, Rotterdam: ErasmusMC

    Google Scholar 

  56. Osterholt KM, Roe LS, Rolls BJ: Incorporation of air into a snack food reduces energy intake. Appetite. 2007, 48: 351-358. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Young LR, Nestle MS: Portion sizes in dietary assessment: issues and policy implications. Nutr Rev. 1995, 53 (6): 149-158.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Hartstein J, Cullen KW, Reynolds KD, Harrell J, Resnicow K, Kennel P: Impact of portion-size control for school a la carte items: changes in kilocalories and macronutrients purchased by middle school students. J Am Diet Assoc. 2008, 108: 140-144. 10.1016/j.jada.2007.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Vermeer WM, Steenhuis IHM, Seidell JC: From the point-of-purchase perspective: A qualitative study of the feasibility of interventions aimed at portion-size. Health Policy. 2009, 90 (1): 73-80. 10.1016/j.healthpol.2008.07.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. O'Dougherty M, Harnack LJ, French SA, Story M, Oakes JM, Jeffery RW: Nutrition labelling and value size pricing at fast-food restaurants: A consumer perspective. Am J Health Promot. 2006, 20 (4): 247-250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Franca Leeuwis for her contribution to this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ingrid HM Steenhuis.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

IS conceived of the study, carried out the literature review and drafted the manuscript. WV participated in the literature review and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Steenhuis, I.H., Vermeer, W.M. Portion size: review and framework for interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 6, 58 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-58

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-58

Keywords